Yonsei Med J.  2018 Sep;59(7):897-903. 10.3349/ymj.2018.59.7.897.

Aberrant Neural Activation Underlying Idiom Comprehension in Korean Children with High Functioning Autism Spectrum Disorder

Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, PURME Foundation NEXON Children's Rehabilitation Hospital, Seoul, Korea.
  • 2Graduate School of Medicine, Yonsei University, Seoul, Korea, Korea.
  • 3Center for Information and Neural Networks, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Osaka, Japan.
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea.
  • 5Yonsei Soul Psychiatric Clinic, Seoul, Korea.
  • 6Division of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Department of Psychiatry, Severance Children's Hospital, Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea. kacheon@yuhs.ac

Abstract

PURPOSE
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by social communication impairments and repetitive behaviors or restricted interests. Impaired pragmatic language comprehension is a universal feature in individuals with ASD. However, the underlying neural basis of pragmatic language is poorly understood. In the present study, we examined neural activation patterns associated with impaired pragmatic language comprehension in ASD, compared to typically developing children (TDC).
MATERIALS AND METHODS
Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) was applied to 15 children with ASD and 18 TDC using the Korean pragmatic language task.
RESULTS
Children with ASD were less accurate than TDC at comprehending idioms, particularly when they were required to interpret idioms with mismatched images (mismatched condition). Children with ASD also showed different patterns of neural activity than TDC in all three conditions (neutral, matched, and mismatched). Specifically, children with ASD showed decreased activation in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) (Brodmann area 47) in the mismatched condition, compared with TDC (IFG; t(31)=3.17, p < 0.001).
CONCLUSION
These results suggest that children with ASD face difficulties in comprehending pragmatic expressions and apply different pragmatic language processes at the neural level.

Keyword

Autism spectrum disorder; fMRI; pragmatics; idiom comprehension; Korean Autism Social Language Task

MeSH Terms

Autism Spectrum Disorder*
Autistic Disorder*
Child*
Comprehension*
Humans
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Neurodevelopmental Disorders
Prefrontal Cortex

Figure

  • Fig. 1 Paradigm design and example of the stimuli used in the KASLAT task. (A) This smells weird. This sentence has a similar meaning to the English expression, “I smell a rat.” Literally, these expressions indicate an awkward smell. However, we understand that something suspicious or unreliable is going on in the social context. (B) Father became a pickled onion. This metaphorically implies an exhausted person. (C) He is digging his own grave. This is a Korean proverb that is used when someone refers to doing something stupid or making mistakes. (D) Crying fist. It refers to a person suffering from extreme anger or frustration. (E) Sit with your two legs stretched forward. (F) She shouted from the top of the mountain. s, sec; KASLAT, Korean Autism Social Language Task. N, neutral; M, matched; MM, mismatched.

  • Fig. 2 Mean RT and accuracy for KASLAT task in the ASD and TDC groups. (A) The RTs (ms) and (B) the percentage of the accuracy of behavior responses were calculated from recorded answers in each condition. Differences were considered statistically significant for *p<0.01, †p<0.05. RT, reaction time; ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typically developing children; N, neutral; M, matched; MM, mismatched; KASLAT, Korean Autism Social Language Task.

  • Fig. 3 Different brain activation maps of ASD and TDC with a KASLAT task. ASD, autism spectrum disorder; TDC, typically developing children; N, neutral; M, matched; MM, mismatched; KASLAT, Korean Autism Social Language Task.


Reference

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®). 5th ed. Arlington (VA): American Psychiatric Association;2013.
2. Caronna EB, Milunsky JM, Tager-Flusberg H. Autism spectrum disorders: clinical and research frontiers. Arch Dis Child. 2008; 93:518–523. PMID: 18305076.
Article
3. Tager-Flusberg H, Paul R, Lord C. Language and communication in autism. In : Volkmar FR, Paul R, Klin A, Cohen D, editors. Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. 3rd ed. Hoboken (NJ): John Wiley & Sons, Inc;2005. p. 335–364.
4. Knaus TA, Silver AM, Lindgren KA, Hadjikhani N, Tager-Flusberg H. fMRI activation during a language task in adolescents with ASD. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008; 14:967–979. PMID: 18954477.
Article
5. Boucher J. Language development in autism. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. 2003; 67(Suppl 1):S159–S163. PMID: 14662187.
Article
6. Boucher J. Research review: structural language in autistic spectrum disorder-characteristics and causes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2012; 53:219–233. PMID: 22188468.
7. Tager-Flusberg H, Joseph RM. Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2003; 358:303–314. PMID: 12639328.
Article
8. Happé FG. Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: a test of relevance theory. Cognition. 1993; 48:101–119. PMID: 8243028.
Article
9. Colle L, Baron-Cohen S, Hill J. Do children with autism have a theory of mind? A non-verbal test of autism vs. specific language impairment. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007; 37:716–723. PMID: 16977496.
Article
10. Groen WB, Zwiers MP, van der Gaag RJ, Buitelaar JK. The phenotype and neural correlates of language in autism: an integrative review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2008; 32:1416–1425. PMID: 18562003.
Article
11. Levorato MC, Cacciari C. Children's comprehension and production of idioms: the role of context and familiarity. J Child Lang. 1992; 19:415–433. PMID: 1527209.
Article
12. Lee SB, Song SH, Ham JH, Song DH, Cheon KA. Idiom comprehension deficits in high-functioning autism spectrum disorder using a Korean autism social language task. Yonsei Med J. 2015; 56:1613–1618. PMID: 26446644.
Article
13. Lo YC, Chou TL, Fan LY, Gau SS, Chiu YN, Tseng WY. Altered structure-function relations of semantic processing in youths with high-functioning autism: a combined diffusion and functional MRI study. Autism Res. 2013; 6:561–570. PMID: 23853172.
Article
14. Schultz RT. Developmental deficits in social perception in autism: the role of the amygdala and fusiform face area. Int J Dev Neurosci. 2005; 23:125–141. PMID: 15749240.
Article
15. Siegal M, Blades M. Language and auditory processing in autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003; 7:378–380. PMID: 12963465.
Article
16. Happé F, Ehlers S, Fletcher P, Frith U, Johansson M, Gillberg C, et al. 'Theory of mind' in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport. 1996; 8:197–201. PMID: 9051780.
Article
17. Castelli F, Frith C, Happé F, Frith U. Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain. 2002; 125(Pt 8):1839–1849. PMID: 12135974.
Article
18. Kim YS, Leventhal BL, Koh YJ, Fombonne E, Laska E, Lim EC, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in a total population sample. Am J Psychiatry. 2011; 168:904–912. PMID: 21558103.
Article
19. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994; 24:659–685. PMID: 7814313.
Article
20. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000; 30:205–223. PMID: 11055457.
21. Cheon KA, Park JI, Koh YJ, Song J, Hong HJ, Kim YK, et al. The social responsiveness scale in relation to DSM IV and DSM5 ASD in Korean children. Autism Res. 2016; 9:970–980. PMID: 27604989.
Article
22. Carr L, Iacoboni M, Dubeau MC, Mazziotta JC, Lenzi GL. Neural mechanisms of empathy in humans: a relay from neural systems for imitation to limbic areas. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2003; 100:5497–5502. PMID: 12682281.
Article
23. Seitz RJ, Schäfer R, Scherfeld D, Friederichs S, Popp K, Wittsack HJ, et al. Valuating other people’s emotional face expression: a combined functional magnetic resonance imaging and electroencephalography study. Neuroscience. 2008; 152:713–722. PMID: 18313858.
Article
24. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J, Perry D. Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain. 2009; 132(Pt 3):617–627. PMID: 18971202.
Article
25. Wang AT, Lee SS, Sigman M, Dapretto M. Neural basis of irony comprehension in children with autism: the role of prosody and context. Brain. 2006; 129(Pt 4):932–943. PMID: 16481375.
Article
26. Tesink CM, Buitelaar JK, Petersson KM, van der Gaag RJ, Kan CC, Tendolkar I, et al. Neural correlates of pragmatic language comprehension in autism spectrum disorders. Brain. 2009; 132(Pt 7):1941–1952. PMID: 19423680.
Article
27. Tesink CM, Petersson KM, van Berkum JJ, van den Brink D, Buitelaar JK, Hagoort P. Unification of speaker and meaning in language comprehension: an FMRI study. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009; 21:2085–2099. PMID: 19016606.
Article
28. St George M, Kutas M, Martinez A, Sereno MI. Semantic integration in reading: engagement of the right hemisphere during discourse processing. Brain. 1999; 122(Pt 7):1317–1325. PMID: 10388797.
Article
29. Rodd JM, Davis MH, Johnsrude IS. The neural mechanisms of speech comprehension: fMRI studies of semantic ambiguity. Cereb Cortex. 2005; 15:1261–1269. PMID: 15635062.
Article
30. Xu J, Kemeny S, Park G, Frattali C, Braun A. Language in context: emergent features of word, sentence, and narrative comprehension. Neuroimage. 2005; 25:1002–1015. PMID: 15809000.
Article
31. Zempleni MZ, Renken R, Hoeks JC, Hoogduin JM, Stowe LA. Semantic ambiguity processing in sentence context: evidence from event-related fMRI. Neuroimage. 2007; 34:1270–1279. PMID: 17142061.
Article
32. McCandliss BD, Cohen L, Dehaene S. The visual word form area: expertise for reading in the fusiform gyrus. Trends Cogn Sci. 2003; 7:293–299. PMID: 12860187.
Article
33. Shaywitz SE, Shaywitz BA. Paying attention to reading: the neurobiology of reading and dyslexia. Dev Psychopathol. 2008; 20:1329–1349. PMID: 18838044.
Article
34. Dehaene S, Pegado F, Braga LW, Ventura P, Nunes Filho G, Jobert A, et al. How learning to read changes the cortical networks for vision and language. Science. 2010; 330:1359–1364. PMID: 21071632.
Article
35. Cohen L, Dehaene S. Specialization within the ventral stream: the case for the visual word form area. Neuroimage. 2004; 22:466–476. PMID: 15110040.
Article
36. Cohen L, Lehéricy S, Henry C, Bourgeois M, Larroque C, Sainte-Rose C, et al. Learning to read without a left occipital lobe: right-hemispheric shift of visual word form area. Ann Neurol. 2004; 56:890–894. PMID: 15562413.
Article
37. Mizuno A, Liu Y, Williams DL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ, Just MA. The neural basis of deictic shifting in linguistic perspective-taking in high-functioning autism. Brain. 2011; 134(Pt 8):2422–2435. PMID: 21733887.
Article
38. Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain. 2004; 127(Pt 8):1811–1821. PMID: 15215213.
Article
39. Hampshire A, Chamberlain SR, Monti MM, Duncan J, Owen AM. The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control. Neuroimage. 2010; 50:1313–1319. PMID: 20056157.
Article
40. Aron AR, Robbins TW, Poldrack RA. Inhibition and the right inferior frontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004; 8:170–177. PMID: 15050513.
Article
Full Text Links
  • YMJ
Actions
Cited
CITED
export Copy
Close
Share
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
Similar articles
Copyright © 2024 by Korean Association of Medical Journal Editors. All rights reserved.     E-mail: koreamed@kamje.or.kr