J Korean Med Sci.  2023 Dec;38(48):e411. 10.3346/jkms.2023.38.e411.

Intelligence-Dependent Differential Effects of Media Exposure on Executive Function Changes in Children: A Population-Based Cohort Study With a Longitudinal Design

Affiliations
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Hallym University Sacred Heart Hospital, Anyang, Korea
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Samsung Medical Center, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, Seoul, Korea

Abstract

Background
Excessive media use is known to be associated with executive dysfunction in children, but it’s unclear whether this exposure can lead to long-term changes of executive function. This study aimed to investigate the association between media exposure and longitudinal changes in executive function within a population-based study, while considering the potential influence of intelligence.
Methods
This study used data from 1,209 participants in the Panel Korea Study for Children. The children’s media exposure was measured at ages 7 and 8, and executive function was evaluated annually from ages 7 to 10 using the Executive Function Difficulty Screening Questionnaire. Participants were grouped by media exposure level (low, medium, or high), and longitudinal changes in executive function were analyzed using linear mixed effects models. Subgroup analysis was conducted to investigate how executive function changes varied based on intelligence within each media exposure group.
Results
Children with high media exposure (n = 97) had severer executive function difficulties than those with low (n = 141) or medium (n = 971) exposure in all waves. The high exposure group demonstrated persistent higher executive function difficulties up to age 10 after controlling for child gender, intelligence, parental education level and maternal depression. Children with intelligence quotient (IQ) ≤ 100 in the medium to high media exposure group had significantly more severe executive function difficulties than those with IQ > 100.
Conclusion
This study provided evidence of a longitudinal negative association between media exposure and executive function. The findings suggest that excessive media exposure may lead to long-term changes in executive function in children and highlight the importance of implementing targeted interventions and educational strategies to mitigate the potential negative effects of excessive media use, particularly for children with lower cognitive abilities.

Keyword

Media Exposure; Executive Function; Longitudinal Study; Screen Time; Cognitive Development

Figure

  • Fig. 1 Study design. Demographic information and maternal depression were assessed at the age of 7. Media usage time was investigated at ages 7 and 8. Intelligence was evaluated at the age of 8. Executive function was assessed four times annually from ages 7 to 10.

  • Fig. 2 Changes in executive function from 7 to 10 years of age. First row: Changes in executive function over time based on media exposure groups (low exposure, medium exposure, and high exposure). Second row: Changes in executive function over time based on intelligence groups (IQ ≤ 100, IQ > 100).IQ = intelligence quotient.

  • Fig. 3 Differing changes over time in executive function between intelligence groups within the same level of media exposure. First row: Executive function changes over time based on intelligence within the high exposure group. Second row: Changes in executive function based on intelligence within the low exposure group.IQ = intelligence quotient.


Reference

1. Kabali HK, Irigoyen MM, Nunez-Davis R, Budacki JG, Mohanty SH, Leister KP, et al. Exposure and use of mobile media devices by young children. Pediatrics. 2015; 136(6):1044–1050. PMID: 26527548.
Article
2. Gentile DA, Coyne S, Walsh DA. Media violence, physical aggression, and relational aggression in school age children: a short-term longitudinal study. Aggress Behav. 2011; 37(2):193–206. PMID: 21274855.
Article
3. Kirkorian HL, Wartella EA, Anderson DR. Media and young children’s learning. Future Child. 2008; 18(1):39–61. PMID: 21338005.
Article
4. Hancox RJ, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Association between child and adolescent television viewing and adult health: a longitudinal birth cohort study. Lancet. 2004; 364(9430):257–262. PMID: 15262103.
Article
5. Chen IH, Chen CY, Pakpour AH, Griffiths MD, Lin CY. Internet-related behaviors and psychological distress among schoolchildren during COVID-19 school suspension. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020; 59(10):1099–1102.e1. PMID: 32615153.
Article
6. Li W, Wang Z, Wang G, Ip P, Sun X, Jiang Y, et al. Socioeconomic inequality in child mental health during the COVID-19 pandemic: first evidence from China. J Affect Disord. 2021; 287:8–14. PMID: 33761325.
Article
7. Panchal U, Salazar de Pablo G, Franco M, Moreno C, Parellada M, Arango C, et al. The impact of COVID-19 lockdown on child and adolescent mental health: systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2023; 32(7):1151–1177. PMID: 34406494.
Article
8. Nathanson AI, Aladé F, Sharp ML, Rasmussen EE, Christy K. The relation between television exposure and executive function among preschoolers. Dev Psychol. 2014; 50(5):1497–1506. PMID: 24447117.
Article
9. Yang X, Chen Z, Wang Z, Zhu L. The relations between television exposure and executive function in Chinese preschoolers: the moderated role of parental mediation behaviors. Front Psychol. 2017; 8:1833. PMID: 29089912.
Article
10. Reed P. Impact of social media use on executive function. Comput Human Behav. 2023; 141:107598.
Article
11. Oh Y, Kim Y, Joung YS. A latent profile analysis of problematic media device use and its association with executive function and behavioral problem among children: a population-based study. Psychiatry Investig. 2021; 18(9):895–903.
Article
12. Soares PS, de Oliveira PD, Wehrmeister FC, Menezes AM, Gonçalves H. Screen time and working memory in adolescents: a longitudinal study. J Psychiatr Res. 2021; 137:266–272. PMID: 33725639.
Article
13. Ardila A, Pineda D, Rosselli M. Correlation between intelligence test scores and executive function measures. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2000; 15(1):31–36. PMID: 14590565.
Article
14. Brydges CR, Reid CL, Fox AM, Anderson M. A unitary executive function predicts intelligence in children. Intelligence. 2012; 40(5):458–469.
Article
15. Arffa S. The relationship of intelligence to executive function and non-executive function measures in a sample of average, above average, and gifted youth. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2007; 22(8):969–978. PMID: 17920807.
Article
16. Mathiassen B, Brøndbo PH, Waterloo K, Martinussen M, Eriksen M, Hanssen-Bauer K, et al. IQ as a predictor of clinician-rated mental health problems in children and adolescents. Br J Clin Psychol. 2012; 51(2):185–196. PMID: 22574803.
Article
17. Koenen KC, Moffitt TE, Roberts AL, Martin LT, Kubzansky L, Harrington H, et al. Childhood IQ and adult mental disorders: a test of the cognitive reserve hypothesis. Am J Psychiatry. 2009; 166(1):50–57. PMID: 19047325.
Article
18. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006; 16(1):17–42. PMID: 16794878.
Article
19. Riggs NR, Jahromi LB, Razza RP, Dillworth-Bart JE, Mueller U. Executive function and the promotion of social–emotional competence. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2006; 27(4):300–309.
Article
20. Zelazo PD, Cunningham WA. Executive function: mechanisms underlying emotion regulation. Gross JJ, editor. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY, USA: The Guilford Press;2007. p. 135–158.
21. Zelazo PD, Qu L, Kesek AC. Hot executive function: emotion and the development of cognitive control. Calkins SD, Bell MA, editors. Child Development at the Intersection of Emotion and Cognition. Washington, D.C., USA: American Psychological Association;2010. p. 97–111.
22. Anderson P. Assessment and development of executive function (EF) during childhood. Child Neuropsychol. 2002; 8(2):71–82. PMID: 12638061.
Article
23. Wang Y, Wu L, Wang L, Zhang Y, Du X, Dong G. Impaired decision-making and impulse control in Internet gaming addicts: evidence from the comparison with recreational Internet game users. Addict Biol. 2017; 22(6):1610–1621. PMID: 27687019.
Article
24. Park CH, Chun JW, Cho H, Jung YC, Choi J, Kim DJ. Is the Internet gaming-addicted brain close to be in a pathological state? Addict Biol. 2017; 22(1):196–205. PMID: 26135331.
Article
25. Wang Y, Wu L, Zhou H, Lin X, Zhang Y, Du X, et al. Impaired executive control and reward circuit in Internet gaming addicts under a delay discounting task: independent component analysis. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2017; 267(3):245–255. PMID: 27506757.
Article
26. Bahk J, Yun SC, Kim YM, Khang YH. Impact of unintended pregnancy on maternal mental health: a causal analysis using follow up data of the Panel Study on Korean Children (PSKC). BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2015; 15(1):85. PMID: 25881099.
Article
27. Lee EJ, Ogbolu Y. Does parental control work with smartphone addiction?: a cross-sectional study of children in South Korea. J Addict Nurs. 2018; 29(2):128–138. PMID: 29864060.
28. Lee C, Lee SJ. Prevalence and predictors of smartphone addiction proneness among Korean adolescents. Child Youth Serv Rev. 2017; 77:10–17.
Article
29. World Health Organization. WHO Guidelines on Physical Activity and Sedentary Behaviour. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organization;2020.
30. Kessler RC, Barker PR, Colpe LJ, Epstein JF, Gfroerer JC, Hiripi E, et al. Screening for serious mental illness in the general population. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003; 60(2):184–189. PMID: 12578436.
Article
31. Min JW, Lee SH. Validation of the K6/K10 scales of psychological distress and their optimal cutoff scores for older Koreans. Int J Aging Hum Dev. 2015; 80(3):264–282. PMID: 26195502.
Article
32. Oh Y, Joung YS, Baek JH, Yoo N. Maternal depression trajectories and child executive function over 9 years. J Affect Disord. 2020; 276:646–652. PMID: 32741750.
Article
33. Song H. Validity of child-adolescent self-reported executive function difficulty screening questionnaire. Korean J Clin Psychol. 2014; 33(1):121–137.
Article
34. Lee J, Hyun S, Choi I. Multi–Factorial Intelligence Test. Seoul, Korea: Insight of Psychology;2014.
35. Oh Y, Kim H, Joung YS. Problematic internet use in children according to maternal depression trajectories: a population-based cohort study with 9-year follow-up. J Psychiatr Res. 2021; 141:364–369. PMID: 34304042.
Article
36. Ttofi MM, Farrington DP, Piquero AR, Lösel F, DeLisi M, Murray J. Intelligence as a protective factor against offending: a meta-analytic review of prospective longitudinal studies. J Crim Justice. 2016; 45:4–18.
Article
37. Schwartz JA, Savolainen J, Aaltonen M, Merikukka M, Paananen R, Gissler M. Intelligence and criminal behavior in a total birth cohort: an examination of functional form, dimensions of intelligence, and the nature of offending. Intelligence. 2015; 51:109–118.
Article
38. Mortensen EL, Michaelsen KF, Sanders SA, Reinisch JM. A dose-response relationship between maternal smoking during late pregnancy and adult intelligence in male offspring. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2005; 19(1):4–11. PMID: 15670102.
Article
39. Lawlor DA, Najman JM, Batty GD, O’Callaghan MJ, Williams GM, Bor W. Early life predictors of childhood intelligence: findings from the Mater-University study of pregnancy and its outcomes. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2006; 20(2):148–162. PMID: 16466433.
Article
40. Sauce B, Liebherr M, Judd N, Klingberg T. The impact of digital media on children’s intelligence while controlling for genetic differences in cognition and socioeconomic background. Sci Rep. 2022; 12(1):7720. PMID: 35545630.
Article
41. Hutton JS, Dudley J, Horowitz-Kraus T, DeWitt T, Holland SK. Associations between screen-based media use and brain white matter integrity in preschool-aged children. JAMA Pediatr. 2020; 174(1):e193869. PMID: 31682712.
Article
42. Madigan S, Browne D, Racine N, Mori C, Tough S. Association between screen time and children’s performance on a developmental screening test. JAMA Pediatr. 2019; 173(3):244–250. PMID: 30688984.
Article
43. Madigan S, McArthur BA, Anhorn C, Eirich R, Christakis DA. Associations between screen use and child language skills: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2020; 174(7):665–675. PMID: 32202633.
Article
44. Fischer KW, Bullock D. Cognitive development in school-age children: conclusions and new directions. Collins WA, editor. Development During Middle Childhood: The Years From Six to Twelve. Washington, D.C., USA: National Academies Press;1984. p. 70–146.
45. Maggi S, Irwin LJ, Siddiqi A, Hertzman C. The social determinants of early child development: an overview. J Paediatr Child Health. 2010; 46(11):627–635. PMID: 20796183.
Article
46. American Academy of Pediatrics Committee on Public Education. American academy of pediatrics: children, adolescents, and television. Pediatrics. 2001; 107(2):423–426. PMID: 11158483.
47. Korea Press Foundation. Children and Media in Korea. Seoul, Korea: Korea Press Foundation;2020.
Full Text Links
  • JKMS
Actions
Cited
CITED
export Copy
Close
Share
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
Similar articles
Copyright © 2024 by Korean Association of Medical Journal Editors. All rights reserved.     E-mail: koreamed@kamje.or.kr