Ann Rehabil Med.  2013 Jun;37(3):347-354. 10.5535/arm.2013.37.3.347.

Sequential Analysis of Postural Control Resource Allocation During a Dual Task Test

Affiliations
  • 1Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Center for Clinical Research, Samsung Medical Center, Sungkyunkwan University School of Medicine, Seoul, Korea.
  • 2Department of Rehabilitation Medicine, Research Institute for Convergence of Biomedical Science and Technology, Pusan National University Yangsan Hospital, Yangsan, Korea. aarondoctor@gmail.com
  • 3Department of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, Samsung Changwon Hospital, Changwon, Korea.
  • 4Department of Motor and Cognitive Function Rehabilitation, National Rehabilitation Research Institute, Seoul, Korea.

Abstract


OBJECTIVE
To investigate the postural control factors influencing the automatic (reflex-controlled) and attentional (high cortical) factors on dual task.
METHODS
We used a dual task model to examine the attentional factors affecting the control of posture, subjecting test subjects to vibration stimulation, one-leg standing and verbal or nonverbal task trials. Twenty-three young, healthy participants were asked to stand on force plates and their centers of pressure were measured during dual task trials. We acquired 15 seconds of data for each volunteer during six dual task trials involving varying task combinations.
RESULTS
We observed significantly different sway patterns between the early and late phases of dual task trials, which probably reflect the attentional demands. Vibration stimulation perturbed sway more during the early than the late phases; with or without vibration stimulation, the addition of secondary tasks decreased sway in all phases, and greater decreases in sway were observed in the late phases, when subjects were assigned nonverbal tasks. Less sway was observed during the nonverbal task in a sequential study.
CONCLUSION
The attentional and automatic factors were analyzed during a sequential study. By controlling the postural control factors, optimal parameters and training methods might be used in clinical applications.

Keyword

Task performance; Analysis; Postural balance; Attention

MeSH Terms

Postural Balance
Posture
Resource Allocation
Task Performance and Analysis
Vibration

Figure

  • Fig. 1 Comparison among the total summation of distance of center of pressure during 6 trials. *Denotes significant differences between different secondary tasks (none and verbal, none and nonverbal). †Denotes significant differences between none and Vibration.

  • Fig. 2 The amount of sway per second with or without vibration during one-leg standing trials. viS, stands for vibration and one-leg standing as the dual task; S, stands for one-leg standing only as the primary task; COP, stands for the center of pressure distance. *Denotes significant differences between viS and S.

  • Fig. 3 The amount of sway per second with secondary tasks during one-leg standing trials. COP, stands for the center of pressure distance; S, stands for one-leg standing as the primary task; Sv, stands for one-leg standing as the primary task and a verbal task as the secondary task; Snv, stands for one-leg standing as the primary task and a nonverbal task as the secondary task. *Denotes significant differences between Sv and Snv. †Denotes significant differences between viS and viSv.

  • Fig. 4 The amount of sway per second with secondary tasks during one-leg standing trials with vibration. COP, stands for the center of pressure distance; viS, stands for vibration and one-leg standing as the primary task; viSv, stands for vibration and one-leg standing as the primary task and a verbal task as the secondary task; viSnv, stands for vibration and one-leg standing as the primary task and a nonverbal task as the secondary task. *Denotes significant differences between viS and viSnv. †Denotes significant differences between viS and viSv. ‡Denotes significant differences between viSv and viSnv.

  • Fig. 5 The amount of sway per second with or without vibration during one-leg standing trials with nonverbal task. COP, stands for the center of pressure distance; viSnv, stands for vibration and one-leg standing as the primary task and a nonverbal task as the secondary task; Snv, stands for one-leg standing as the primary task and a nonverbal task as the secondary task.

  • Fig. 6 The amount of sway per second with or without vibration during one-leg standing trials with verbal task. COP, stands for the center of pressure distance; viSv, stands for vibration and one-leg standing as the primary task and a verbal task as the secondary task; Sv, stands for one-leg standing as the primary task and a verbal task as the secondary task.


Reference

1. Kejonen P, Kauranen K, Ahasan R, Vanharanta H. Motion analysis measurements of body movements during standing: association with age and sex. Int J Rehabil Res. 2002; 25:297–304. PMID: 12451305.
Article
2. Maylor EA, Wing AM. Age differences in postural stability are increased by additional cognitive demands. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 1996; 51:P143–P154. PMID: 8620354.
Article
3. Pellecchia GL. Postural sway increases with attentional demands of concurrent cognitive task. Gait Posture. 2003; 18:29–34. PMID: 12855298.
Article
4. Swan L, Otani H, Loubert PV. Reducing postural sway by manipulating the difficulty levels of a cognitive task and a balance task. Gait Posture. 2007; 26:470–474. PMID: 17161948.
Article
5. Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture. 2002; 16:1–14. PMID: 12127181.
Article
6. Collins JJ, De Luca CJ. Open-loop and closed-loop control of posture: a random-walk analysis of center-of-pressure trajectories. Exp Brain Res. 1993; 95:308–318. PMID: 8224055.
Article
7. Thompson C, Belanger M, Fung J. Effects of bilateral Achilles tendon vibration on postural orientation and balance during standing. Clin Neurophysiol. 2007; 118:2456–2467. PMID: 17897877.
Article
8. Teasdale N, Bard C, LaRue J, Fleury M. On the cognitive penetrability of posture control. Exp Aging Res. 1993; 19:1–13. PMID: 8444263.
Article
9. Eklund G. General features of vibration-induced effects on balance. Ups J Med Sci. 1972; 77:112–124. PMID: 4262735.
Article
10. Eklund G. Further studies of vibration-induced effects on balance. Ups J Med Sci. 1973; 78:65–72. PMID: 4702333.
11. Brumagne S, Janssens L, Janssens E, Goddyn L. Altered postural control in anticipation of postural instability in persons with recurrent low back pain. Gait Posture. 2008; 28:657–662. PMID: 18541428.
Article
12. Brumagne S, Janssens L, Knapen S, Claeys K, Suuden-Johanson E. Persons with recurrent low back pain exhibit a rigid postural control strategy. Eur Spine J. 2008; 17:1177–1184. PMID: 18594876.
Article
13. De Nunzio AM, Nardone A, Picco D, Nilsson J, Schieppati M. Alternate trains of postural muscle vibration promote cyclic body displacement in standing parkinsonian patients. Mov Disord. 2008; 23:2186–2193. PMID: 18785234.
Article
14. Hasson CJ, Van Emmerik RE, Caldwell GE, Haddad JM, Gagnon JL, Hamill J. Influence of embedding parameters and noise in center of pressure recurrence quantification analysis. Gait Posture. 2008; 27:416–422. PMID: 17604174.
Article
15. Michel-Pellegrino V, Amoud H, Hewson DJ, Duchene J. Identification of a degradation in postural equilibrium invoked by different vibration frequencies on the tibialis anterior tendon. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. 2006; 1:4047–4050. PMID: 17946218.
Article
16. Lajoie Y, Teasdale N, Bard C, Fleury M. Attentional demands for static and dynamic equilibrium. Exp Brain Res. 1993; 97:139–144. PMID: 8131825.
Article
17. McIlroy WE, Norrie RG, Brooke JD, Bishop DC, Nelson AJ, Maki BE. Temporal properties of attention sharing consequent to disturbed balance. Neuroreport. 1999; 10:2895–2899. PMID: 10549793.
Article
18. Binder LM, Johnson-Greene D. Observer effects on neuropsychological performance: a case report. Clin Neuropsychol. 1995; 9:74–78.
Article
19. Yantz CL, McCaffrey RJ. Effects of parental presence and child characteristics on children's neuropsychological test performance: third party observer effect confirmed. Clin Neuropsychol. 2009; 23:118–132. PMID: 18609333.
Article
20. Rothmayr C, Baumann O, Endestad T, Rutschmann RM, Magnussen S, Greenlee MW. Dissociation of neural correlates of verbal and non-verbal visual working memory with different delays. Behav Brain Funct. 2007; 3:56. PMID: 17958919.
Article
21. Smith EE, Jonides J, Koeppe RA. Dissociating verbal and spatial working memory using PET. Cereb Cortex. 1996; 6:11–20. PMID: 8670634.
Article
22. Smith EE, Jonides J, Marshuetz C, Koeppe RA. Components of verbal working memory: evidence from neuroimaging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998; 95:876–882. PMID: 9448254.
Article
23. Yardley L, Gardner M, Leadbetter A, Lavie N. Effect of articulatory and mental tasks on postural control. Neuroreport. 1999; 10:215–219. PMID: 10203311.
Article
24. Boggio PS, Ferrucci R, Rigonatti SP, Covre P, Nitsche M, Pascual-Leone A, et al. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on working memory in patients with Parkinson's disease. J Neurol Sci. 2006; 249:31–38. PMID: 16843494.
Article
25. Ohn SH, Park CI, Yoo WK, Ko MH, Choi KP, Kim GM, et al. Time-dependent effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on the enhancement of working memory. Neuroreport. 2008; 19:43–47. PMID: 18281890.
Article
26. Salavati M, Mazaheri M, Negahban H, Ebrahimi I, Jafari AH, Kazemnejad A, et al. Effect of dual-tasking on postural control in subjects with nonspecific low back pain. Spine (Phila Pa 1976). 2009; 34:1415–1421. PMID: 19478663.
Article
27. Vette AH, Masani K, Sin V, Popovic MR. Posturographic measures in healthy young adults during quiet sitting in comparison with quiet standing. Med Eng Phys. 2010; 32:32–38. PMID: 19884033.
Article
28. Laufer Y. Effect of cognitive demand during training on acquisition, retention and transfer of a postural skill. Hum Mov Sci. 2008; 27:126–141. PMID: 17942179.
Article
29. Andersson G, Hagman J, Talianzadeh R, Svedberg A, Larsen HC. Effect of cognitive load on postural control. Brain Res Bull. 2002; 58:135–139. PMID: 12121823.
Article
30. Vuillerme N, Nafati G. How attentional focus on body sway affects postural control during quiet standing. Psychol Res. 2007; 71:192–200. PMID: 16215747.
Article
31. Doumas M, Smolders C, Krampe RT. Task prioritization in aging: effects of sensory information on concurrent posture and memory performance. Exp Brain Res. 2008; 187:275–281. PMID: 18273609.
Article
32. Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M. Attentional demands and postural control: the effect of sensory context. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2000; 55:M10–M16. PMID: 10719767.
33. Shumway-Cook A, Woollacott M, Kerns KA, Baldwin M. The effects of two types of cognitive tasks on postural stability in older adults with and without a history of falls. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 1997; 52:M232–M240. PMID: 9224435.
Article
34. Fuller GF. Falls in the elderly. Am Fam Physician. 2000; 61:2159–2168. 2173–2174. PMID: 10779256.
35. Li KZ, Lindenberger U, Freund AM, Baltes PB. Walking while memorizing: age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychol Sci. 2001; 12:230–237. PMID: 11437306.
Article
36. Swan L, Otani H, Loubert PV, Sheffert SM, Dunbar GL. Improving balance by performing a secondary cognitive task. Br J Psychol. 2004; 95(Pt 1):31–40. PMID: 15005866.
Article
Full Text Links
  • ARM
Actions
Cited
CITED
export Copy
Close
Share
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
Similar articles
Copyright © 2024 by Korean Association of Medical Journal Editors. All rights reserved.     E-mail: koreamed@kamje.or.kr