J Korean Med Sci.  2014 Oct;29(10):1416-1424. 10.3346/jkms.2014.29.10.1416.

Neural Substrates of Hanja (Logogram) and Hangul (Phonogram) Character Readings by Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

Affiliations
  • 1Neuroscience Research Institute, Gachon University, Incheon, Korea. neurokim@gachon.ac.kr
  • 2Department of Psychology, Yeungnam University, Kyongsan, Korea.
  • 3Kansei Fukushi Research Institute, Tohoku Fukushi University, Sendai, Japan.

Abstract

The two basic scripts of the Korean writing system, Hanja (the logography of the traditional Korean character) and Hangul (the more newer Korean alphabet), have been used together since the 14th century. While Hanja character has its own morphemic base, Hangul being purely phonemic without morphemic base. These two, therefore, have substantially different outcomes as a language as well as different neural responses. Based on these linguistic differences between Hanja and Hangul, we have launched two studies; first was to find differences in cortical activation when it is stimulated by Hanja and Hangul reading to support the much discussed dual-route hypothesis of logographic and phonological routes in the brain by fMRI (Experiment 1). The second objective was to evaluate how Hanja and Hangul affect comprehension, therefore, recognition memory, specifically the effects of semantic transparency and morphemic clarity on memory consolidation and then related cortical activations, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (Experiment 2). The first fMRI experiment indicated relatively large areas of the brain are activated by Hanja reading compared to Hangul reading. The second experiment, the recognition memory study, revealed two findings, that is there is only a small difference in recognition memory for semantic transparency, while for the morphemic clarity was much larger between Hanja and Hangul. That is the morphemic clarity has significantly more effect than semantic transparency on recognition memory when studies by fMRI in correlation with behavioral study.

Keyword

Neural Substrates of Language; Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging; Functional Neuroimaging; Hanja and Hangul

MeSH Terms

Adult
Brain/*physiology
Brain Mapping/*methods
Brain Waves/*physiology
Female
Humans
Magnetic Resonance Imaging
Male
Neurolinguistic Programming
Recognition (Psychology)/physiology
Writing

Figure

  • Fig. 1 Neural hypothesis of the character processing of logographic and non-logographic languages. ILF, inferior longitudinal fasciculus; SLF, superior longitudinal fasciculus; IFO, inferior fronto-occipital fasciculus; SFO, superior fronto-occipital fasciculus.

  • Fig. 2 A set of Hanja and Hangul scripts used in the word recognition experiment. The left panel illustrates a Hanja set and at the right a Hangul set. Within each activation block, a word was displayed for 1 sec, resulting a total of 30 sec for 30 words given per block.

  • Fig. 3 Experimental sequence of the functional MRI recognition memory study. Names were categorized according to morphemic clarity and semantic transparency (e.g., HT-HMC, LT-HMC, HT-LMC, and LT-LMC) and presented in random order. Each block was composed of three phases: reading (2 sec), rehearsal (4 sec) and button press (2 sec).

  • Fig. 4 Experiment 1: Two sets of experimentally obtained images demonstrating the two potential pathways; the grapheme and phoneme dorsal pathways. Illustrations (A) and (B) depict Hangul>Baseline and Hanja>Baseline results. Illustration (C) is Hangul>Hanja where the angular gyrus (AG) is activated with Hangul (Hangul>Hanja), suggesting the involvement of this area for grapheme to phoneme conversion, while in (D) shows the case of Hanja>Hangul and depicts hypothetical pathway which involves both Broca's area (BA) and premotor area (PM) for Hanja reading.

  • Fig. 5 Experiment 2: Behavioral results of recognition memory. Recognition memory data of the four cases (HT-HMC, LT-HMC, HT-LMC, and LT-LMC) was collected 1 min (at the left), 10 min (at the middle), and 120 min (at the right) after the fMRI experiment. As seen, the difference between the transparency groups (HT-HMC to LT-HMC) was small, while the difference between the morphemic clarity (MC) groups (HT-HMC to HT-LMC and LT-HMC to LT-LMC) was large.

  • Fig. 6 Experiment 2: Comparison of cortical activation during Hanja vs Hangul names encoding. Results from the fMRI study are shown in three axial view images (A, B, and C), one sagittal (D) and one coronal (E), respectively. The areas most activated with Hanja are the left parahippocampal area (A, B, D, and E), posterior temporal cortex (C) and the lateral superior parietal cortex (LSPC) (D). This result is consistent with the notion that better long-term memory recognition for Hanja is supported by increased activation of the hippocampal area.


Reference

1. Tan LH, Laird AR, Li K, Fox PT. Neuroanatomical correlates of phonological processing of Chinese characters and alphabetic words: a meta-analysis. Hum Brain Mapp. 2005; 25:83–91.
2. Lee KM. Functional MRI comparison between reading ideographic and phonographic scripts of one language. Brain Lang. 2004; 91:245–251.
3. Libben G. Semantic transparency in the processing of compounds: consequences for representation, processing, and impairment. Brain Lang. 1998; 61:30–44.
4. Amenta S, Crepaldi D. Morphological processing as we know it: an analytical review of morphological effects in visual word identification. Front Psychol. 2012; 3:232.
5. Perfetti CA, Hart L. The lexical quality hypothesis. In : Verhoeven L, Carsten E, Reitsma P, editors. Precursors of functional literacy. Amsterdam; Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company;2002. p. 67–86.
6. Perfetti C. Reading ability: lexical quality to comprehension. Sci Stud Read. 2007; 11:357–383.
7. Paulesu E, McCrory E, Fazio F, Menoncello L, Brunswick N, Cappa SF, Cotelli M, Cossu G, Corte F, Lorusso M, et al. A cultural effect on brain function. Nat Neurosci. 2000; 3:91–96.
8. Chen C, Xue G, Mei L, Chen C, Dong Q. Cultural neurolinguistics. Prog Brain Res. 2009; 178:159–171.
9. Price CJ. The anatomy of language: a review of 100 fMRI studies published in 2009. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2010; 1191:62–88.
10. Goodale MA, Milner AD. Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends Neurosci. 1992; 15:20–25.
11. Tokunaga H, Nishikawa T, Ikejiri Y, Nakagawa Y, Yasuno F, Hashikawa K, Nishimura T, Sugita Y, Takeda M. Different neural substrates for Kanji and Kana writing: a PET study. Neuroreport. 1999; 10:3315–3319.
12. Harpaz Y, Levkovitz Y, Lavidor M. Lexical ambiguity resolution in Wernicke's area and its right homologue. Cortex. 2009; 45:1097–1103.
13. Nishitani N, Schürmann M, Amunts K, Hari R. Broca's region: from action to language. Physiology (Bethesda). 2005; 20:60–69.
14. Hickok G, Poeppel D. Dorsal and ventral streams: a framework for understanding aspects of the functional anatomy of language. Cognition. 2004; 92:67–99.
15. Magnuson JS, Tanenhaus MK, Aslin RN, Dahan D. The time course of spoken word learning and recognition: studies with artificial lexicons. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2003; 132:202–227.
16. Sakurai Y, Momose T, Iwata M, Sudo Y, Ohtomo K, Kanazawa I. Different cortical activity in reading of Kanji words, Kana words and Kana nonwords. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2000; 9:111–115.
17. Ino T, Tokumoto K, Usami K, Kimura T, Hashimoto Y, Fukuyama H. Longitudinal fMRI study of reading in a patient with letter-by-letter reading. Cortex. 2008; 44:773–781.
18. Kwon M, Kim JS, Lee JH, Sim H, Nam K, Park H. Double dissociation of Hangul and Hanja reading in Korean patients with stroke. Eur Neurol. 2005; 54:199–203.
19. Sakurai Y, Terao Y, Ichikawa Y, Ohtsu H, Momose T, Tsuji S, Mannen T. Pure alexia for kana. Characterization of alexia with lesions of the inferior occipital cortex. J Neurol Sci. 2008; 268:48–59.
20. Sakurai Y, Asami M, Mannen T. Alexia and agraphia with lesions of the angular and supramarginal gyri: evidence for the disruption of sequential processing. J Neurol Sci. 2010; 288:25–33.
21. Lucas TH 2nd, McKhann GM 2nd, Ojemann GA. Functional separation of languages in the bilingual brain: a comparison of electrical stimulation language mapping in 25 bilingual patients and 117 monolingual control patients. J Neurosurg. 2004; 101:449–457.
22. Usui K, Ikeda A, Takayama M, Matsuhashi M, Satow T, Begum T, Kinoshita M, Miyamoto S, Hashimoto N, Nagamine T, et al. Processing of Japanese morphogram and syllabogram in the left basal temporal area: electrical cortical stimulation studies. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2005; 24:274–283.
23. Gabrieli JD, Brewer JB, Poldrack RA. Images of medial temporal lobe functions in human learning and memory. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 1998; 70:275–283.
24. Schacter DL, Wagner AD. Medial temporal lobe activations in fMRI and PET studies of episodic encoding and retrieval. Hippocampus. 1999; 9:7–24.
25. Kirchhoff BA, Wagner AD, Maril A, Stern CE. Prefrontal-temporal circuitry for episodic encoding and subsequent memory. J Neurosci. 2000; 20:6173–6180.
26. Otten LJ, Henson RN, Rugg MD. Depth of processing effects on neural correlates of memory encoding: relationship between findings from across- and within-task comparisons. Brain. 2001; 124:399–412.
27. Carr VA, Viskontas IV, Engel SA, Knowlton BJ. Neural activity in the hippocampus and perirhinal cortex during encoding is associated with the durability of episodic memory. J Cogn Neurosci. 2010; 22:2652–2662.
28. Diemand-Yauman C, Oppenheimer DM, Vaughan EB. Fortune favors the bold (and the Italicized): effects of disfluency on educational outcomes. Cognition. 2011; 118:111–115.
Full Text Links
  • JKMS
Actions
Cited
CITED
export Copy
Close
Share
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
Similar articles
Copyright © 2024 by Korean Association of Medical Journal Editors. All rights reserved.     E-mail: koreamed@kamje.or.kr