J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc.  2015 Aug;54(3):282-290. 10.4306/jknpa.2015.54.3.282.

The Effect of Depression in Decision Making Process : Based on Quantitative Methodology

Affiliations
  • 1The Catholic University of Korea College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea.
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, The Catholic University of Korea College of Medicine, Seoul, Korea. alberto@catholic.ac.kr

Abstract

The increasing number of patients with depression is a serious social issue in contemporary Korean society. To fully understand the pathophysiology of depression, this paper reviewed how depression affects the decision making process of humans. Various recent studies in behavioral economics, mathematics, medicine, and neurobiology have shown how humans make decisions and how emotional disturbances, such as depressive disorder, affect this process. There has been great progress in behavioral economics during this decade, and numerous experiments have been designed to evaluate decision making process in humans. In general, economic decision making is evaluated using the Iowa Gambling Task, and social decision making is assessed using the ultimatum game. Numerous research studies have analyzed the performance and reaction of patients with depression in these games. As a result of the advancement of neurophysiology, research has successfully identified the part of the brain that causes the specific results of tests being conducted on patients with depression. Meanwhile, computational neuroscientists have established decision making models based on bayesian framework. These models also match with the neuroanatomy. Although a large part remains unclear, researchers look forward to achieving a better understanding in depression by analyzing the distinct patterns of responses that patients under depression show in the experiment of behavioral economics.

Keyword

Depression; Decision making; Behavioral economics; Interdisciplinary studies

MeSH Terms

Affective Symptoms
Brain
Decision Making*
Depression*
Depressive Disorder
Economics, Behavioral
Gambling
Humans
Interdisciplinary Studies
Iowa
Mathematics
Neuroanatomy
Neurobiology
Neurophysiology

Figure

  • Fig. 1 (A) Hypothetical value functions based on calculation (dotted line) and based on feeling (solid line). (B) Hypothetical calculated (dotted line) and affect-rich (solid line) probability weighting functions. Adapted from Hsee % Rottenstreich, 2004; 133:23-30 with permission.6)

  • Fig. 2 Partially Observable Markov Decision Processes (POMDPs). Adapted from Paulus % Yu, 2012;16:476-483 with permission. 12)

  • Fig. 3 Suggested mapping of elements of the model to components of the cortex-basal ganglia network. Adapted from Rao, 2010;4:146 with permission.29) STN : Subthalamic nucleus, GPE : Globus pallidus externa, Gpi : Globus pallidus interna, SNr : Substance nigra resticlar formation, SNc : Striatum-substnata nigra compacta, VTA : Ventral tegmental area, TD : Temporal difference.


Reference

1. Bechara A. The role of emotion in decision-making: evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain Cogn. 2004; 55:30–40.
Article
2. Rangel A, Camerer C, Montague PR. A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008; 9:545–556.
Article
3. Paulus MP. Decision-making dysfunctions in psychiatry--altered homeostatic processing? Science. 2007; 318:602–606.
Article
4. Walton ME, Behrens TE, Noonan MP, Rushworth MF. Giving credit where credit is due: orbitofrontal cortex and valuation in an uncertain world. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011; 1239:14–24.
Article
5. Mellers BA, Biagini K. Similarity and choice. Psychol Rev. 1994; 101:505–518.
Article
6. Hsee CK, Rottenstreich Y. Music, pandas, and muggers: on the affective psychology of value. J Exp Psychol Gen. 2004; 133:23–30.
Article
7. Rottenstreich Y, Hsee CK. Money, kisses, and electric shocks: on the affective psychology of risk. Psychol Sci. 2001; 12:185–190.
Article
8. Rudebeck PH, Walton ME, Smyth AN, Bannerman DM, Rushworth MF. Separate neural pathways process different decision costs. Nat Neurosci. 2006; 9:1161–1168.
Article
9. Maule AJ, Hockey GR, Bdzola L. Effects of time-pressure on decision-making under uncertainty: changes in affective state and information processing strategy. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2000; 104:283–301.
Article
10. Tversky A, Kahneman D. Availability: a heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cogn Psychol. 1973; 5:207–232.
Article
11. Rilling JK, Sanfey AG. The neuroscience of social decision-making. Annu Rev Psychol. 2011; 62:23–48.
Article
12. Paulus MP, Yu AJ. Emotion and decision-making: affect-driven belief systems in anxiety and depression. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012; 16:476–483.
Article
13. Mayberg HS. Limbic-cortical dysregulation: a proposed model of depression. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1997; 9:471–481.
Article
14. Yang Y, Raine A. Prefrontal structural and functional brain imaging findings in antisocial, violent, and psychopathic individuals: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. 2009; 174:81–88.
Article
15. Winkielman P, Knutson B, Paulus M, Trujillo JL. Affective influence on judgments and decisions: moving towards core mechanisms. Rev Gen Psychol. 2007; 11:179–192.
Article
16. Ekman P. Are there basic emotions? Psychol Rev. 1992; 99:550–553.
Article
17. Lerner JS, Keltner D. Fear, anger, and risk. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2001; 81:146–159.
Article
18. Kahneman D. A perspective on judgment and choice: mapping bounded rationality. Am Psychol. 2003; 58:697–720.
Article
19. Kahneman D, Tversky A. Prospect theory: an analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica. 1979; 47:263–291.
Article
20. Mukherjee K. A dual system model of preferences under risk. Psychol Rev. 2010; 117:243–255.
Article
21. Kusev P, van Schaik P. Preferences under risk: content-dependent behavior and psychological processing. Front Psychol. 2011; 2:269.
Article
22. Kusev P, van Schaik P, Ayton P, Dent J, Chater N. Exaggerated risk: prospect theory and probability weighting in risky choice. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2009; 35:1487–1505.
Article
23. Vlaev I. Inconsistency in risk preferences: a psychophysical anomaly. Front Psychol. 2011; 2:304.
Article
24. Platt ML, Huettel SA. Risky business: the neuroeconomics of decision making under uncertainty. Nat Neurosci. 2008; 11:398–403.
Article
25. Battaglia PW, Jacobs RA, Aslin RN. Bayesian integration of visual and auditory signals for spatial localization. J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis. 2003; 20:1391–1397.
Article
26. Green DM, Swets JA. Signal detection theory and psychophysics. Los Altos, CA: John Wiley & Sons;1974. p. 10–94.
27. Körding KP, Wolpert DM. Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature. 2004; 427:244–247.
Article
28. Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Walton ME, Rushworth MF. Learning the value of information in an uncertain world. Nat Neurosci. 2007; 10:1214–1221.
Article
29. Rao RP. Decision making under uncertainty: a neural model based on partially observable markov decision processes. Front Comput Neurosci. 2010; 4:146.
Article
30. Pizzagalli DA. Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2011; 36:183–206.
Article
31. Bechara A, Damasio AR, Damasio H, Anderson SW. Insensitivity to future consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition. 1994; 50:7–15.
Article
32. Smoski MJ, Lynch TR, Rosenthal MZ, Cheavens JS, Chapman AL, Krishnan RR. Decision-making and risk aversion among depressive adults. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. 2008; 39:567–576.
Article
33. Han G, Klimes-Dougan B, Jepsen S, Ballard K, Nelson M, Houri A, et al. Selective neurocognitive impairments in adolescents with major depressive disorder. J Adolesc. 2012; 35:11–20.
Article
34. von Helversen B, Wilke A, Johnson T, Schmid G, Klapp B. Performance benefits of depression: sequential decision making in a healthy sample and a clinically depressed sample. J Abnorm Psychol. 2011; 120:962–968.
Article
35. Bearden JN, Rapoport A, Murphy RO. Sequential observation and selection with rank-dependent payoffs: an experimental test. Manag Sci. 2006; 52:1437–1449.
Article
36. Strunk DR, Adler AD. Cognitive biases in three prediction tasks: a test of the cognitive model of depression. Behav Res Ther. 2009; 47:34–40.
Article
37. Bless H, Bohner G, Schwarz N, Strack F. Mood and persuasion: a cognitive response analysis. Pers Soc Psychol Bull. 1990; 16:331–345.
38. Gleicher F, Weary G. Effect of depression on quantity and quality of social inferences. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1991; 61:105–114.
Article
39. Andrews PW, Thomson JA Jr. The bright side of being blue: depression as an adaptation for analyzing complex problems. Psychol Rev. 2009; 116:620–654.
Article
40. Jollant F, Bellivier F, Leboyer M, Astruc B, Torres S, Verdier R, et al. Impaired decision making in suicide attempters. Am J Psychiatry. 2005; 162:304–310.
Article
41. Foti D, Hajcak G. Depression and reduced sensitivity to non-rewards versus rewards: evidence from event-related potentials. Biol Psychol. 2009; 81:1–8.
Article
42. Bronisch T, Wittchen HU. Suicidal ideation and suicide attempts: comorbidity with depression, anxiety disorders, and substance abuse disorder. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 1994; 244:93–98.
Article
43. Chase HW, Camille N, Michael A, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Sahakian BJ. Regret and the negative evaluation of decision outcomes in major depression. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2010; 10:406–413.
Article
44. Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, Ratner KG, Jahn AL. Increased perceived stress is associated with blunted hedonic capacity: potential implications for depression research. Behav Res Ther. 2007; 45:2742–2753.
Article
45. Nakano M, Matsuo K, Nakashima M, Matsubara T, Harada K, Egashira K, et al. Gray matter volume and rapid decision-making in major depressive disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2014; 48:51–56.
Article
46. Forbes EE, Christopher May J, Siegle GJ, Ladouceur CD, Ryan ND, Carter CS, et al. Reward-related decision-making in pediatric major depressive disorder: an fMRI study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006; 47:1031–1040.
Article
47. Güth W, Schmittberger R, Schwarze B. An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. J Econ Behav Organ. 1982; 3:367–388.
Article
48. Scheele D, Mihov Y, Schwederski O, Maier W, Hurlemann R. A negative emotional and economic judgment bias in major depression. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2013; 263:675–683.
Article
49. Destoop M, Schrijvers D, De Grave C, Sabbe B, De Bruijn ER. Better to give than to take? Interactive social decision-making in severe major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. 2012; 137:98–105.
Article
50. Kerr N, Dunbar RI, Bentall RP. Theory of mind deficits in bipolar affective disorder. J Affect Disord. 2003; 73:253–259.
Article
51. Inoue Y, Yamada K, Kanba S. Deficit in theory of mind is a risk for relapse of major depression. J Affect Disord. 2006; 95:125–127.
Article
52. Kampman O, Poutanen O. Can onset and recovery in depression be predicted by temperament? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. 2011; 135:20–27.
Article
53. Harlé KM, Allen JJ, Sanfey AG. The impact of depression on social economic decision making. J Abnorm Psychol. 2010; 119:440–446.
Article
54. Wang Y, Zhou Y, Li S, Wang P, Wu GW, Liu ZN. Impaired social decision making in patients with major depressive disorder. BMC Psychiatry. 2014; 14:18.
Article
55. Crockett MJ, Clark L, Hauser MD, Robbins TW. Serotonin selectively influences moral judgment and behavior through effects on harm aversion. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010; 107:17433–17438.
Article
Full Text Links
  • JKNA
Actions
Cited
CITED
export Copy
Close
Share
  • Twitter
  • Facebook
Similar articles
Copyright © 2024 by Korean Association of Medical Journal Editors. All rights reserved.     E-mail: koreamed@kamje.or.kr